ArtsAutosBooksBusinessEducationEntertainmentFamilyFashionFoodGamesGenderHealthHolidaysHomeHubPagesPersonal FinancePetsPoliticsReligionSportsTechnologyTravel

What Happens to Our Memory As We Get Older?

Updated on September 29, 2015
John Paul Quester profile image

John Paul is a retired academic with a background in psychology and philosophy.

Graphical mnemonic devices from works by Giordano Bruno (1548-1600)
Graphical mnemonic devices from works by Giordano Bruno (1548-1600) | Source

In two previous hubs, I addressed the relationship between aging and intelligence, and aging and creativity, respectively. I propose here to discuss age related changes in memory. These changes are cause of concern to some older people, being regarded as harbingers of dementia. To allay these concerns, it may be helpful to review some empirical findings regarding the normal, non-pathological changes in memory that accompany aging.

Varieties of memory

A diffuse opinion among non specialists is that aging inevitably leads to an across the board decline in the ability to remember. In fact, the effects of age are highly dependent upon the type of memory task being performed. Some tasks show large age related impairments, whereas no decline and even improvement is observed in others well into late middle age. This in turn points to the existence of different types of memory.

Memory can be differentiated on the basis of the kinds of storehouses’ that hold our recollections: sensory memory, short term memory, and long term memory. Further, memories can be declarative or non-declarative (procedural), implicit or explicit, retrospective or prospective; these terms are defined below. Memory can also be characterized in terms of the operations involved in it, namely the encoding, storage (retention), and retrieval of mnemonic material. Let' s begin by looking at the various ‘storehouses’ of memory.

Sensory memory and aging

Sensory memory, the first to be engaged in any memory formation, temporarily retains in the mind 'copies' of incoming sensory data (especially visual and auditory) for the duration necessary to extract the information they carry. Laboratory studies show that sensory memory is very little affected by age, and we can therefore dispense with discussing it further.

Short term memory and aging

Short-term memory (STM) refers to the conscious retention of a current event for a brief period (such as listening over the phone for a meeting time and place, and holding it in mind until it can be written down).

Originally, STM was regarded as a mechanism that only allows the temporary and passive storage of information, as in the above example. More recently, the scope of STM was broadened to include ‘working’ memory, which involves the simultaneous maintenance and active manipulation of information. For example, when multiplying two two-digit numbers, one must retain each of the two numbers in mind while performing the multiplication operation upon them.

Aging affects passive short term memory only modestly. However, working memory is more negatively affected. In sum, older adults are more impaired on tasks that require both the processing and storage of information than on tasks requiring only storage. Also, age-related differences increase as the processing required becomes more demanding.

Long term memory and aging

Let’s turn next to long-term memory (LTM), the storehouse of information covering a widely ranging time period. LTM can retain recent data, such as where you left your book half an hour ago, decades-old information, such as what happened at your tenth birthday party, and the meaning of words and the knowledge of facts acquired over a lifetime of learning.

LTM can be further characterized in a number of ways. One important distinction is between explicit memory and implicit memory. Implicit memory processes are effortless and unintentional (e.g., when we quickly and effortlessly recognize a friend in a crowd); the operations of explicit memory are intentional and require a conscious effort (e.g., when we try to remember an address that was given to us yesterday). Aging affects these two forms of LTM differently: implicit memory declines much less than explicit memory.

Another important distinction is between declarative and non-declarative (or ‘procedural’) memories. Declarative memory refers mostly to memories that can be 'declared', i.e. stated verbally. In contrast, non-declarative memory consists largely of memory for activities or procedures, e.g., mowing a lawn, playing the piano, driving a car, etc.

Procedural memory holds up well with age. As for declarative memories, a further distinction is made between ‘episodic’ memory, which involves personal memories tied to a particular time and place (e.g., remembering who came to your dinner party at your cottage a month ago), and ‘semantic’ memory, constituted by the knowledge of general facts (e.g., naming the author of Great Expectations, or defining a given word) which do not require the recollection of the time and place in which they were originally acquired.

Like the explicit/implicit distinction discussed above, episodic and semantic memories are differentially affected by aging. Overall, semantic memory holds up comparatively well. Episodic memory, however, can decline significantly. Age related changes in episodic memory are induced by reduced functionality of memory processes at encoding (the acquiring of information), during the retention/maintenance/storage stage, and at retrieval of the information.

Episodic memory can also be assessed by asking people of different ages to remember events in their personal past. The study of autobiographical memory is complicated by the fact that unlike mnemonic materials created in the laboratory, researchers cannot verify the accuracy of most autobiographical memories. Nevertheless, the study of autobiographical memory is important because it can tell us a great deal about the aging process.

Unsurprisingly, in general, regardless of age people tend to better remember recent personal events than earlier events. However, an important exception emerges when we consider the whole lifespan of the individual. We remember especially well memories acquired between the ages of l0 and 30. This effect, known as the ‘reminiscence bump’, has been attributed, simply put, to the fact that many highly significant events occur to people during the age of 10-30 years, which is why they are so well remembered.

This helps in assessing a frequent claim by older people: that they remember well events which occurred many years ago, but often cannot remember what happened to them just a few days earlier. Whereas some of these claims are justified by the reminiscence bump, in assessing them more generally it is essential to consider that they are often based on wrong comparisons. For example, someone might compare their recollection of their first child’s birth many years ago, with remembering what they had for dinner 4 days ago: they remember the former so well, and the latter so poorly! However, by so doing they are comparing memories of very different significance. Now equalize their import: would they recollect better what they had for dinner three days ago or what they ate the corresponding day three years earlier?

False Memory and aging

Older adults are more vulnerable to misinformation – that is, to incorporating into their memory for an original event material that does not belong there. Ironically, they are also more confident than younger adults in the accuracy of these false memories!

Prospective memory and aging

When we think of memory we refer typically to the recollection of past events. But there is also a ‘prospective’ memory, which refers to remembering to execute an intended action at some specific time in the future. Prospective memory tasks are essential for maintaining the quality of older adults' lives, for they include, e.g., remembering to pay bills on time, or remembering to take medication. Indeed, serious difficulties in remembering to carry out tasks such as these can jeopardize independent living. Happily, older adults performed just as well and often better than younger adults in several (though not all) laboratory and real-life studies involving prospective memory.

Can memory in old age be improved?

The results discussed above show that as we get older memory losses of varying degrees of severity occur for some varieties of memory. Accordingly, a question of interest is whether special training could improve older adults' memory performance. The results of some studies are encouraging, as the performance of older adults on a variety of memory tasks was significantly enhanced as a result of mnemonic training. Most mnemonic methods work about equally well. One of the oldest is the so called ‘loci’ mnemonic, which consists of associating images along a familiar route with to-be-remembered items.

These results are important also because they show that even in old age memory and its associated brain areas retain a certain amount of plasticity, and possess often untapped potential.

However, although older adults benefit greatly from intense mnemonic training, the latter is unlikely to completely remove age differences in memory performance (and younger adults can improve their memory to an even greater extent than the elderly when given mnemonic training).

© 2014 John Paul Quester

working

This website uses cookies

As a user in the EEA, your approval is needed on a few things. To provide a better website experience, hubpages.com uses cookies (and other similar technologies) and may collect, process, and share personal data. Please choose which areas of our service you consent to our doing so.

For more information on managing or withdrawing consents and how we handle data, visit our Privacy Policy at: https://corp.maven.io/privacy-policy

Show Details
Necessary
HubPages Device IDThis is used to identify particular browsers or devices when the access the service, and is used for security reasons.
LoginThis is necessary to sign in to the HubPages Service.
Google RecaptchaThis is used to prevent bots and spam. (Privacy Policy)
AkismetThis is used to detect comment spam. (Privacy Policy)
HubPages Google AnalyticsThis is used to provide data on traffic to our website, all personally identifyable data is anonymized. (Privacy Policy)
HubPages Traffic PixelThis is used to collect data on traffic to articles and other pages on our site. Unless you are signed in to a HubPages account, all personally identifiable information is anonymized.
Amazon Web ServicesThis is a cloud services platform that we used to host our service. (Privacy Policy)
CloudflareThis is a cloud CDN service that we use to efficiently deliver files required for our service to operate such as javascript, cascading style sheets, images, and videos. (Privacy Policy)
Google Hosted LibrariesJavascript software libraries such as jQuery are loaded at endpoints on the googleapis.com or gstatic.com domains, for performance and efficiency reasons. (Privacy Policy)
Features
Google Custom SearchThis is feature allows you to search the site. (Privacy Policy)
Google MapsSome articles have Google Maps embedded in them. (Privacy Policy)
Google ChartsThis is used to display charts and graphs on articles and the author center. (Privacy Policy)
Google AdSense Host APIThis service allows you to sign up for or associate a Google AdSense account with HubPages, so that you can earn money from ads on your articles. No data is shared unless you engage with this feature. (Privacy Policy)
Google YouTubeSome articles have YouTube videos embedded in them. (Privacy Policy)
VimeoSome articles have Vimeo videos embedded in them. (Privacy Policy)
PaypalThis is used for a registered author who enrolls in the HubPages Earnings program and requests to be paid via PayPal. No data is shared with Paypal unless you engage with this feature. (Privacy Policy)
Facebook LoginYou can use this to streamline signing up for, or signing in to your Hubpages account. No data is shared with Facebook unless you engage with this feature. (Privacy Policy)
MavenThis supports the Maven widget and search functionality. (Privacy Policy)
Marketing
Google AdSenseThis is an ad network. (Privacy Policy)
Google DoubleClickGoogle provides ad serving technology and runs an ad network. (Privacy Policy)
Index ExchangeThis is an ad network. (Privacy Policy)
SovrnThis is an ad network. (Privacy Policy)
Facebook AdsThis is an ad network. (Privacy Policy)
Amazon Unified Ad MarketplaceThis is an ad network. (Privacy Policy)
AppNexusThis is an ad network. (Privacy Policy)
OpenxThis is an ad network. (Privacy Policy)
Rubicon ProjectThis is an ad network. (Privacy Policy)
TripleLiftThis is an ad network. (Privacy Policy)
Say MediaWe partner with Say Media to deliver ad campaigns on our sites. (Privacy Policy)
Remarketing PixelsWe may use remarketing pixels from advertising networks such as Google AdWords, Bing Ads, and Facebook in order to advertise the HubPages Service to people that have visited our sites.
Conversion Tracking PixelsWe may use conversion tracking pixels from advertising networks such as Google AdWords, Bing Ads, and Facebook in order to identify when an advertisement has successfully resulted in the desired action, such as signing up for the HubPages Service or publishing an article on the HubPages Service.
Statistics
Author Google AnalyticsThis is used to provide traffic data and reports to the authors of articles on the HubPages Service. (Privacy Policy)
ComscoreComScore is a media measurement and analytics company providing marketing data and analytics to enterprises, media and advertising agencies, and publishers. Non-consent will result in ComScore only processing obfuscated personal data. (Privacy Policy)
Amazon Tracking PixelSome articles display amazon products as part of the Amazon Affiliate program, this pixel provides traffic statistics for those products (Privacy Policy)
ClickscoThis is a data management platform studying reader behavior (Privacy Policy)